Stimulus Size (stimulus + size)

Distribution by Scientific Domains


Selected Abstracts


Stimulus size and the variability of the threshold response in the central and peripheral visual field

OPHTHALMIC AND PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS, Issue 6 2002
L. S. Kim
Purpose:, The investigation of the peripheral visual field has shown considerable interest for the investigation of field loss attributed to anticonvulsant therapy. The purpose was to determine the within-visit between-subject, the between-visit between-subject, and the between-location variability of the threshold response in the normal eye with increase in stimulus eccentricity out to 60° as a function of stimulus size. Methods:, Forty-eight normal subjects attended for a total of three visits (mean age = 49.5 years, SD = 18.9, range 22,84 years). At the first visit, one randomly assigned eye of each subject was examined with the Humphrey Field Analyzer 750 (Carl Zeiss, Jena, Germany) and the Full Threshold algorithm using Programs 30,2 and 60,4 and stimulus sizes III and V. The combination of stimulus size and of program, and the order of the combination within- and between-sessions, were randomized for each subject. The results of the first visit were considered as a familiarization period and were discarded. The protocol at the second and third visits was identical to that at the first visit for each subject. Results:, The ratio of the SD of the group mean sensitivity was determined at each stimulus location for stimulus size III compared with stimulus size V for Programs 30,2 and 60,4 at visit 3. The SDs were greater than unity for Program 30,2 (p < 0.0001) and for Program 60,4 (p < 0.0001) indicating greater variability for the size III stimulus. The SDs were also greater than unity for the central inner zone (p < 0.0001), central outer zone (p < 0.0001) and peripheral inner zone (p < 0.0001). The ratios in the peripheral outer zone were not quite greater than unity (p = 0.054). The ratios increased with increase in eccentricity by up to 2.7 times between 15° and 30° eccentricity and by up to 2.7 times between 30° and 60° eccentricity. The group mean ratio did not vary significantly between the two visits for Program 30,2 stimulus size III (p = 0.563), Program 60,4 stimulus size III (p = 0.935) and for Program 60,4 stimulus size V (p = 0.005). However, the group mean SD was lower at visit 3 compared with visit 2 for Program 30,2 stimulus size V (p = 0.0004). The SDs associated with the extreme peripheral locations in the superior and nasal fields were smaller for stimulus size III because the threshold was frequently attenuated by lid and facial contour. Conclusions:, Considerably narrower confidence limits for normality for the peripheral regions of Program 30,2 and for 60,4 are demonstrated with the use of Goldmann size V. [source]


Arousal and attention: Picture size and emotional reactions

PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, Issue 5 2007
Maurizio Codispoti
Abstract Building on the assumption that the motivational relevance of an emotional scene depends on contextual factors such as proximity or stimulus size, the present study examined the effects of picture size on emotional perception using autonomic, facial, and subjective reactions. Affective changes were measured while participants viewed pictures presented in small, medium, and large sizes and varying in affective picture content. Whereas affective modulation of heart rate and Corrugator Supercilii muscle activity were not modulated by picture size, emotional modulation of skin conductance was absent for the smallest stimuli and increased linearly for the medium and largest stimulus sizes. Stimulus size modulated sympathetic changes possibly related to activation of the strategic motivational systems and action preparation. In contrast, responses related to orienting, categorization, and communicative functions did not covary with picture size. [source]


Analysis and use of FMRI response delays

HUMAN BRAIN MAPPING, Issue 2 2001
Ziad S. Saad
Abstract In this study, we implemented a new method for measuring the temporal delay of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) responses and then estimated the statistical distribution of response delays evoked by visual stimuli (checkered annuli) within and across voxels in human visual cortex. We assessed delay variability among different cortical sites and between parenchyma and blood vessels. Overall, 81% of all responsive voxels showed activation in phase with the stimulus while the remaining voxels showed antiphase, suppressive responses. Mean delays for activated and suppressed voxels were not significantly different (P < 0.001). Cortical flat maps showed that the pattern of activated and suppressed voxels was dynamically induced and depended on stimulus size. Mean delays for blood vessels were 0.7,2.4 sec longer than for parenchyma (P < 0.01). However, both parenchyma and blood vessels produced responses with long delays. We developed a model to identify and quantify different components contributing to variability in the empirical delay measurements. Within-voxel changes in delay over time were fully accounted for by the effects of empirically measured fMRI noise with virtually no measurable variability associated with the stimulus-induced response itself. Across voxels, as much as 47% of the delay variance was also the result of fMRI noise, with the remaining variance reflecting fixed differences in response delay among brain sites. In all cases, the contribution of fMRI noise to the delay variance depended on the noise power at the stimulus frequency. White noise models significantly underestimated the fMRI noise effects. Hum. Brain Mapping 13:74,93, 2001. © 2001 Wiley-Liss, Inc. [source]


Stimulus size and the variability of the threshold response in the central and peripheral visual field

OPHTHALMIC AND PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS, Issue 6 2002
L. S. Kim
Purpose:, The investigation of the peripheral visual field has shown considerable interest for the investigation of field loss attributed to anticonvulsant therapy. The purpose was to determine the within-visit between-subject, the between-visit between-subject, and the between-location variability of the threshold response in the normal eye with increase in stimulus eccentricity out to 60° as a function of stimulus size. Methods:, Forty-eight normal subjects attended for a total of three visits (mean age = 49.5 years, SD = 18.9, range 22,84 years). At the first visit, one randomly assigned eye of each subject was examined with the Humphrey Field Analyzer 750 (Carl Zeiss, Jena, Germany) and the Full Threshold algorithm using Programs 30,2 and 60,4 and stimulus sizes III and V. The combination of stimulus size and of program, and the order of the combination within- and between-sessions, were randomized for each subject. The results of the first visit were considered as a familiarization period and were discarded. The protocol at the second and third visits was identical to that at the first visit for each subject. Results:, The ratio of the SD of the group mean sensitivity was determined at each stimulus location for stimulus size III compared with stimulus size V for Programs 30,2 and 60,4 at visit 3. The SDs were greater than unity for Program 30,2 (p < 0.0001) and for Program 60,4 (p < 0.0001) indicating greater variability for the size III stimulus. The SDs were also greater than unity for the central inner zone (p < 0.0001), central outer zone (p < 0.0001) and peripheral inner zone (p < 0.0001). The ratios in the peripheral outer zone were not quite greater than unity (p = 0.054). The ratios increased with increase in eccentricity by up to 2.7 times between 15° and 30° eccentricity and by up to 2.7 times between 30° and 60° eccentricity. The group mean ratio did not vary significantly between the two visits for Program 30,2 stimulus size III (p = 0.563), Program 60,4 stimulus size III (p = 0.935) and for Program 60,4 stimulus size V (p = 0.005). However, the group mean SD was lower at visit 3 compared with visit 2 for Program 30,2 stimulus size V (p = 0.0004). The SDs associated with the extreme peripheral locations in the superior and nasal fields were smaller for stimulus size III because the threshold was frequently attenuated by lid and facial contour. Conclusions:, Considerably narrower confidence limits for normality for the peripheral regions of Program 30,2 and for 60,4 are demonstrated with the use of Goldmann size V. [source]


Measuring contrast sensitivity with inappropriate optical correction*

OPHTHALMIC AND PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS, Issue 6 2000
Russell L. Woods
Summary Spatial frequency-selective minima (notches) in the contrast sensitivity function (CSF) because of defocus can mimic those that occur with ocular disease. We examined the influence of measurement conditions on CSF shape in simulated clinical testing. CSF notches occurred with almost all levels of defocus for all subjects. Multiple notches were found under some conditions. Notches were found with defocus as small as 0.50 D. Effects of induced astigmatism depended on the orientation of the target. Notches were apparent in defocus conditions after stimulus size and room illuminance were modified and when subjects had insufficient accommodation to compensate for hypermetropic defocus. The equivalent of notches was not noted with the Pelli-Robson chart. As defocus-induced CSF notches may be mistaken for functional loss, careful refractive correction should be conducted prior to clinical or experimental CSF measurement, even at low spatial frequencies. [source]


Arousal and attention: Picture size and emotional reactions

PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, Issue 5 2007
Maurizio Codispoti
Abstract Building on the assumption that the motivational relevance of an emotional scene depends on contextual factors such as proximity or stimulus size, the present study examined the effects of picture size on emotional perception using autonomic, facial, and subjective reactions. Affective changes were measured while participants viewed pictures presented in small, medium, and large sizes and varying in affective picture content. Whereas affective modulation of heart rate and Corrugator Supercilii muscle activity were not modulated by picture size, emotional modulation of skin conductance was absent for the smallest stimuli and increased linearly for the medium and largest stimulus sizes. Stimulus size modulated sympathetic changes possibly related to activation of the strategic motivational systems and action preparation. In contrast, responses related to orienting, categorization, and communicative functions did not covary with picture size. [source]


Acquired loss of chromatic sensitivity

ACTA OPHTHALMOLOGICA, Issue 2009
J BARBUR
Purpose A range of ophthalmic and neurological conditions cause diminished visual performance, even when the subject is often unaware of any problems and the loss of vision remains undetected in conventional perimetry and visual acuity tests. The extent to which detection of acquired colour vision loss can revealed in subclinical cases and distinguished from congenital loss has been investigated. Methods Over 400 subjects with congenital and acquired colour vision loss have been examined using conventional colour screening methods. In addition, the loss of yellow / blue and red / green chromatic sensitivity has been quantified using the CAD test (http://www.caa.co.uk/docs/33/200904.pdf). Those investigated included subjects with diseases of the retina and / or the optic nerve as well as patients with selective damage to central visual pathways. Patients with various stages of glaucoma, photoreceptor dystrophies, diabetes, optic neuritis, age-related macular degeneration as well as tobacco and alcohol toxicity have been examined. Results Algorithms developed for analysis of colour vision loss and automatic classification of congenital and / or acquired colour deficiency will be described. In acquired deficiency, the loss of chromatic sensitivity tends to affect both the rg and the yb channels. Significant differential effects have, however, been observed in relation to stimulus size, retinal location and state of light adaptation. Conclusion The findings from these studies show that in the majority of these conditions, the loss of chromatic sensitivity is the most sensitive measure of early changes in diseases of the eye. [source]


Optimal stimulus size maps in the primary visual cortex revealed by optical imaging in cats

ACTA OPHTHALMOLOGICA, Issue 2007
C CASANOVA
Purpose: It is well known that the responses of cells in the primary visual cortex depend on stimulus size. While the stimulus-size dependency has been well documented at the cellular level, nothing is known about its consequences on global functional maps. Methods: Optical imaging of intrinsic signals in the primary visual cortex was carried out in anesthetized cats. Stimuli consisted of 0.75 to 0.1 cycles per degree square-wave gratings drifting in 8 directions at 2 to 4 Hz and were presented monocularly. Responses were obtained for different stimulus diameters (3 to 50 deg, and a full screen condition). Results: The minimal visual stimulation necessary to activate areas 17 and 18 was around 3 and 6 deg. in diameter respectively. The activation area of cortex (10-30 mm2) was dependent of the eccentricity (0 to 30 deg). The pixelwise measure of the signal magnitude in this area showed a modular organisation uncorrelated with the orientation map and stable in time: Half of the pixels had a maximum activation for the full screen stimulation (full field facilitation) and the other half attained their maximum for diameters about 15 and 30 deg of diameter in area 17 and 18 respectively (full field suppression). The suppression by the full screen stimulation was around 30% in both areas. Conclusions: Thus, the maximum activation revealed by optical imaging necessitates the stimulation of a much larger spatial area than that observed with single cells. This difference is likely due to the fact that this method reflects in- and out going signals and reveals activity of adjacent neurons being part of intra-cortical and thalamo-cortical circuits. Supp: NSERC and CIHR. [source]


Arousal and attention: Picture size and emotional reactions

PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, Issue 5 2007
Maurizio Codispoti
Abstract Building on the assumption that the motivational relevance of an emotional scene depends on contextual factors such as proximity or stimulus size, the present study examined the effects of picture size on emotional perception using autonomic, facial, and subjective reactions. Affective changes were measured while participants viewed pictures presented in small, medium, and large sizes and varying in affective picture content. Whereas affective modulation of heart rate and Corrugator Supercilii muscle activity were not modulated by picture size, emotional modulation of skin conductance was absent for the smallest stimuli and increased linearly for the medium and largest stimulus sizes. Stimulus size modulated sympathetic changes possibly related to activation of the strategic motivational systems and action preparation. In contrast, responses related to orienting, categorization, and communicative functions did not covary with picture size. [source]


The study of chromatic and achromatic VEP in the first year of life

ACTA OPHTHALMOLOGICA, Issue 2009
M TEKAVCIC POMPE
Purpose To study chromatic and achromatic VEP responses in the first year of life. Methods In 30 babies aged 2 to 12 months VEP to chromatic and achromatic stimuli were binocularly recorded. Chromatic VEP were recorded to isoluminant red-green and blue-yellow stimulus. The stimulus was a circle composed of horizontal sinusoidal gratings with 90 % chromatic contrast. Two stimulus sizes (7 deg and 21 deg) were used. The stimulus was presented in an onset-offset mode (on , 300ms, off , 700 ms). Achromatic VEP were recorded to black and white checkerboard (50, square size), which was presented in pattern reversal (reversal VEP) and onset-offset (onset VEP) mode. VEP were recorded from Oz (mid occipital) position and the reference was at Fz. Results Chromatic VEP responses were present in all babies, except youngest two (2 months old). The positive wave (P) amplitude to red-green and blue-yellow stimulus increased with age when using both stimulus sizes. Achromatic VEP responses were present in all babies. P100 wave (pattern reversal stimulation) showed longer latency in youngest babies (p=0.045), whereas its amplitude did not change throughout the first year of life. C1 wave (onset-offset stimulation) showed longer latency in youngest babies (p=0.0039), whereas its amplitude decreased with increasing age (p=0.0087). Conclusion Chromatic VEP responses can be recorded in babies after the age of 3 months and show marked maturational changes throughout the first year of life. [source]