Hunting Strategies (hunting + strategy)

Distribution by Scientific Domains


Selected Abstracts


Mammalian Diet and Broad Hunting Strategy of the Dingo (Canis familiaris dingo) in the Wet Tropical Rain Forests of Northeastern Australia,

BIOTROPICA, Issue 2 2001
Karl Vernes
ABSTRACT The diet of dingoes (Canis familiaris dingo) in the Australian Wet Tropics was examined by analyzing 383 dingo scats collected throughout the region for the presence of mammal prey remains. The scats yielded 29 native and 4 introduced mammal prey species from 14 families. The most important species in terms of percentage occurrence in the scats were Melomys cervinipes (22.2%), hoodon macrourus (17.0%), Perameles nasuta (12.5%), and Thylogale stigmatica (12.5%). The most important families were Muridae (37.1%), Peramelidae (29.5%), and Macropodidae (25.8%). Examination of small-scale habitat preferences revealed species that preferentially use the forest edge ranked significantly higher in the diet than those that do not, and species that are terrestrial ranked higher in the diet than those that are arboreal. Relative abundance was also a significant factor in the ranked dietary occurrence of each species, with abundant species ranked significantly higher than those that are less abundant. These results suggest that dingoes in the Australian Wet Tropics are opportunistic predators of a wide variety of mammal species, with abundant terrestrial and forest edge-dwelling taxa the most susceptible to predation. [source]


Hunting strategies and foraging performance of the short-toed eagle in the Dadia-Lefkimi-Soufli National Park, north-east Greece

JOURNAL OF ZOOLOGY, Issue 3 2010
D. E. Bakaloudis
Abstract The foraging performance and the hunting strategies of foraging short-toed eagles Circaetus gallicus were studied in Dadia-Lefkimi-Soufli National Park during 1996,1998. A general linear model analysis showed that the eagle's hunting mode was related to wind velocity. At low wind speeds, the eagles more frequently soared and/or hovered, whereas on windy days, they hung more frequently than soared or hovered. Individuals appear to compensate for the high-cost foraging method (hovering) with a high capture rate or a low capture rate with low-cost foraging methods (soaring and hanging). In addition, their foraging activities exhibited two patterns. In the early (April) and late (September) breeding season, eagles foraged mainly during midday, while from May to August eagles foraged largely during the morning and a little during the afternoon, reflecting to some extent the diurnal activity of prey (reptiles) throughout the breeding season. Short-toed eagles tended to forage for longer as the breeding season progressed, peaking during August due to additional food requirements before autumn migration. Following a mixed foraging strategy throughout the breeding season, short-toed eagles increased their hunting efficiency, which may benefit increased breeding success and energy reserves for migration. [source]


Avoiding predators at night: antipredator strategies in red-tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ruficaudatus)

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY, Issue 6 2007
Claudia Fichtel
Abstract Although about one-third of all primate species are nocturnal, their antipredator behavior has rarely been studied directly. Crypsis and a solitary lifestyle have traditionally been considered to be the main adaptive antipredator strategies of nocturnal primates. However, a number of recent studies have revealed that nocturnal primates are not as cryptic and solitary as previously suggested. Thus, the antipredator strategies available for diurnal primates that rely on early detection and warning of approaching predators may also be available to nocturnal species. In order to shed additional light on the antipredator strategies of nocturnal primates, I studied pair-living red-tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ruficaudatus) in Western Madagascar. In an experimental field study I exposed adult sportive lemurs that lived in pairs and had offspring to playbacks of vocalizations of their main aerial and terrestrial predators, as well as to their own mobbing calls (barks) given in response to disturbances at their tree holes. I documented the subjects' immediate behavioral responses, including alarm calls, during the first minute following a playback. The sportive lemurs did not give alarm calls in response to predator call playbacks or to playbacks with barks. Other behavioral responses, such as gaze and escape directions, corresponded to the hunting strategies of the two classes of predators, suggesting that the corresponding vocalizations were correctly categorized. In response to barks, they scanned the ground and fled. Because barks do not indicate any specific threats, they are presumably general alarm calls. Thus, sportive lemurs do not rely on early warning of acoustically simulated predators; rather, they show adaptive escape strategies and use general alarm calls that are primarily directed toward the predator but may also serve to warn kin and pair-partners. Am. J. Primatol. 69:611,624, 2007. © 2007 Wiley-Liss, Inc. [source]


Feigning death in the Central American cichlid Parachromis friedrichsthalii

JOURNAL OF FISH BIOLOGY, Issue 3 2005
M. Tobler
Feigning death, a hunting strategy in which a healthy individual acts as if it was dead to trick prey into its reach, is reported for a population of the Central American cichlid Parachromis friedrichsthalii. Possible mechanisms leading to the evolution of such a behaviour are discussed. [source]