Capture Success (capture + success)

Distribution by Scientific Domains


Selected Abstracts


Prey size selection in piscivorous pikeperch (Stizostedion lucioperca) includes active prey choice

ECOLOGY OF FRESHWATER FISH, Issue 4 2002
H. Turesson
Abstract,,,Knowledge of the mechanisms behind prey selection in piscivorous fish is important for our understanding of the dynamics of freshwater systems. Prey selection can involve active predator choice or be a passive process. We experimentally studied size-selectivity in pikeperch, feeding on roach and rudd. When given a choice of different prey sizes, pikeperch selected small prey. Passive selection mechanisms (encounter rate, capture success and satiation) could not fully explain the pattern of diet choice. Instead, behavioural analysis revealed that the pikeperch actively selected small-sized prey. Optimal foraging theory, predicting that predators will choose prey sizes giving highest energy return per time spent foraging, is assumed to explain active choice. We measured handling times for a range of prey sizes and found that the most profitable sizes were also the chosen ones, both in experiments and in the field. This suggests that pikeperch choose their prey to maximise energy intake per unit time. [source]


Predation by the tropical plant Utricularia foliosa

FRESHWATER BIOLOGY, Issue 11 2006
LIZANDRO SANABRIA-ARANDA
Summary 1. We examined the prey captured by individual plants of the tropical carnivorous plant Utricularia foliosa, located in different areas along a creek in the Colombian Amazon and the zooplankton and macroinvertebrate communities associated with the plants. The aims were: (i) to determine whether bladders of different sizes within each plant catch different numbers of prey or exploit different size ranges and types of prey, (ii) if the quantity and composition of prey captured varies temporally and/or spatially and (iii) if the plant has evolved effective mechanisms of attracting prey. 2. Utricularia foliosa captured the most abundant species of macroinvertebrates associated with the plant. Larger bladders captured more, larger and more diverse prey. However, benefits of the extra prey caught by large bladders were not offset by the greater cost of producing bladders larger than approximately 1650 ,m. 3. The number of prey captured was higher in those plants with more carbohydrates per bladder and with a higher ratio of antenna size/bladder length. The antennae enhance capture success by offering the prey a favourable substratum that exploits their natural locomotor and feeding behaviour. However, although carbohydrates may lure prey, carbohydrate production was not a strategy of the plant to enhance the capture of prey, because the amount of carbohydrates in the bladder was related to the abundance of periphyton. [source]


Observations on feed size and capture success in the larval butterfly splitfin (Ameca splendens Miller & Fitzsimons, 1971, Pisces: Goodeidae) reared on zooplankton

JOURNAL OF APPLIED ICHTHYOLOGY, Issue 3 2007
F. Peņa-Aguado
Summary In this study, we quantified the feeding behaviour (encounter, attack, capture. and ingestion) of larval A. splendens on micro-crustacean prey [cladocerans: Alona rectangula, Simocephalus vetulus (separately neonates and adults), Ceriodaphnia dubia, Daphnia pulex (juveniles), Moina macrocopa and ostracods: Heterocypris incongruens]. Although we initially (first 4 weeks) offered rotifers (Brachionus calyciflorus and B. patulus), they were not consumed by the larvae and hence observations with these prey were discontinued. Feeding behaviour was observed during the first 10 weeks. Fifteen observations were made with each prey species (seven diets × four replicates). Experiments were conducted in 50 ml transparent containers with 20 ml fish-conditioned water into which one fry was introduced. Before introducing the fish, 20 individuals of a given cladoceran prey species or 50 individuals of a rotifer prey species were introduced. Until the fourth week, we used 20 ml of medium and thereafter 30 ml, but the prey density used remained constant (1 ind. ml,1). Observations (10 min per fry per cladoceran replicate) were taken under a stereomicroscope (20×) for the first 2 weeks and later with a lamp and a magnifying lens. The number of encounters (E), attacks (A), captures (C) and ingestions (I) were recorded. During the study period, there was a 60% increase in gape size but only a 30% increase in body length. The number of encounters of larval A. splendens was highest (192) on M. macrocopa and lowest (29) on ostracods and adult S. vetulus (59). The inverse relationship between capture success and prey size was more pronounced during the latter half of the study period. Compared with all the other prey types offered, A. splendens fed maximally on M. macrocopa, which therefore could be a suitable diet for the larval rearing of this fish species. [source]


Scale-dependent responses to forest cover displayed by frugivore bats

OIKOS, Issue 11 2008
Naiara Pinto
Despite vast evidence of species turnover displayed by Neotropical bat communities in response to forest fragmentation, the exact shape of the relationship between fragment area and abundance for individual bat species is still unclear. Bats' ample variation in diet, morphology, and movement behaviour can potentially influence species' perception of the landscape. Thus, studies describing fragment area at a single spatial scale may fail to capture the amount of forest available from the perspective of individual bat species. In the present paper, we study the influence of forest cover on bats inhabiting a fragmented forest in Mexico, focusing on some of the most common frugivore species: Artibeus jamaicensis, Carollia spp. (C. brevicauda/C. perspicillata) and Sturnira spp. (S. lilium/S. ludovici). We quantified forest cover at scales ranging from 50 to 2000,m, and measured the influence of forest cover on bat capture success, a surrogate for abundance. The three species displayed positive and significant scale-dependent associations with forest cover. Abundance of A. jamaicensis increased with forest cover measured at scales ranging between 500 and 2000,m, while Carollia spp. responded more strongly to variation in forest cover measured at scales 100,500,m. For Sturnira spp., abundance was a function of presence of creeks near mist-netting sites, and amount of secondary forest present at a 200,m scale. The observed variation in responses to forest cover can be explained in light of interspecific differences in diet, home range, and body size. Our results illustrate a method for measuring the effect of forest fragmentation on mobile species and suggest that changes in abundance in fragmented landscapes emerge from the interaction between species' traits and landscape structure. [source]


Active selection for large guppies, Poecilia reticulata, by the pike cichlid, Crenicichla saxatilis

OIKOS, Issue 3 2004
Jan Johansson
Size-selective predation has been proposed to be one important evolutionary force shaping life-history traits in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Populations living in the presence of the ring-tailed pike cichlid (Crenicichlasaxatilis) are smaller, mature earlier, allocate more energy to offspring and get more and smaller young than guppies in localities without Crenicichla. We investigated if Crenicichlasaxatilis is a size-selective predator, if the selectivity is a result of active choice and if the optimal prey size can be explained according to an optimal foraging model. In single-prey experiments we quantified the predators' pre-capture costs (time), capture success, and post-capture costs (time) for four different prey sizes spanning from 10 to 40 mm total length. To see which of the components of the prey cycle the predator takes into account for its choice, we then predicted prey values and optimal prey size with 6 different models that included one or more of the prey cycle components. In two multiple prey experiments, the cichlids were given the choice of the two and four different prey sizes simultaneously. Crenicichlasaxatilis actively selected the largest guppies in both cases. The three prey-value functions that included handling time (post-capture cost) did not accurately predict the prey choice. Instead the prey-value functions that took into account pre-capture cost (approach and attack time) were able to correctly predict the choice of the largest guppy size, suggesting that pre-capture costs may be more important than post-capture costs for prey choice in Crenicichlasaxatilis. The study confirms that Crenicichlasaxatilis is a size-selective predator selecting large guppies, while earlier evidence for selectivity for large prey in Crenicichla cichlids has been weak and equivocal. Our result strengthen the possibility that size-selective predation is a mechanism in life-history evolution in guppies. [source]


Effect of abiotic factors on the foraging strategy of the orb-web spider Argiope keyserlingi (Araneae: Araneidae)

AUSTRAL ECOLOGY, Issue 6 2003
M. E. Herberstein
Abstract Environmental conditions such as light level, background contrast and temperature might influence a spider's prey capture success and risk of predation. Thus it may often be advantageous for spiders to adjust web-building behaviour in response to variation in these environmental conditions. This hypothesis was examined in a study of the construction of webs and web decorations (conspicuous strands of silk at the hub of the web) of the orb-web spider Argiope keyserlingi. Web decorations are thought to have one or more separate functions. They may attract prey, deter predators or advertise the web to oncoming birds, thus preventing web damage. In this series of experiments, relationships between weather parameters and the construction of webs and web decorations were considered. In complementary laboratory experiments, A. keyserlingi spiders were exposed to two different light levels (700 and 90 lx), background contrasts (black and white) and temperature conditions (20 and 26°C). Of the available weather parameters, only temperature was significantly related to web decorating behaviour but not to web size. In the laboratory, temperature also influenced web-decorating behaviour, and spiders in dim light (700 lx) constructed larger webs and longer decorations. Background contrast did not significantly alter web size or web decorations. These data suggest that when prey availability is reduced at low temperatures, spiders may use web decorations to attract prey to the web. Similarly, in dim light, spiders may build more and larger decorations to increase the visual signal to approaching prey or to advertise the web to oncoming birds. [source]